Skip to Main Navigation
placeholder placeholder placeholder
placeholder
placeholder placeholder
Literacy Journal Logo - Click to return to home page
placeholder

placeholder

 
placeholder placeholder
fall 2003 web forum readings

Here are some excerpts to help with our discussion. It is not necessary to read these articles to join the discussion. This list is by no means complete and we welcome any suggestions you have for other readings.

The first two are from Focus on Basics and are about qualitative and quantitative research. They give us a little background on the difference.

The third piece is a description of the IALS from the IALS web page.

There are two pieces by Mary Hamilton.

The first appears in the journal and in it she discusses which literacies are included in the IALS data and which are missed. In the second excerpt, she describes the ‘vernacular’ literacies she found missing from the IALS data.

We have included three excerpts from Thomas Sticht.

The first is the one that is printed in the journal and looks at the difference between self-assessment and performance assessment in the IALS.

The second addresses the point that Susan Sussman makes in the interview “that the IALS test requires that people have an 80 per cent probability of responding correctly to questions at a given literacy level, in order to count as fitting into that literacy level. One of the designers of the test, according to Sussman, has said that a more realistic “pass” rate would be 50 per cent.”

In the third, he discusses how the IALS data can be used to refute “the concerns for global competitiveness that some policy-oriented reports have used to focus adult literacy education on workforce development.”

The last three pieces are excerpt from the debate between Stan Jones and Brian Street about whether or not the IALS privileges certain literacies and if it does, what that means to literacy learners and workers.

  1. from Research with Words: Qualitative Inquiry
    by Glynda Hull
    FOCUS ON BASICS. Vol 1, Issue A • Feb 97
    Full text available at http://gseweb.harvard.edu/~ncsall/fob/1997/hull.htm
  2. from Understanding Quantitative Research about Adult Literacy
    by Thomas Valentine
    FOCUS ON BASICS, Vol 1, Issue A • Feb 97
    Full text available at http://gseweb.harvard.edu/~ncsall/fob/1997/valen.htm
  3. from Reading the Future: A Portrait of Literacy In Canada
    Backgrounder
    Full text available at http://www.nald.ca/NLS/ials/ialsreps/ialsbk1.htm
  4. from Privileged Literacies: Policy, Institutional Process and the Life of the IALS
    by Mary Hamilton, Language and Education, 2001, 15 (2, 3): 178-196
  5. from Becoming expert: using ethnographies of everyday learning to inform the education of adults
    Papers from the 28th Annual SCUTREA Conference
    Research, Teaching and Learning: making connections in the education of adults
    by Mary Hamilton, Literacy Research Group, Department of Educational Research, Lancaster University, UK.
    http://www.leeds.ac.uk/educol/documents/000000719.htm
  6. from The International Adult Literacy Survey: How well does it represent the literacy abilities of adults?
    by Thomas G. Sticht, The Canadian Journal for the Study of Adult Education, Vol, 15, No. 2 November, 2001, 15(2): pp19-36
  7. from How Many Low Literate Adults Are There in Canada, the United States, and United Kingdom? Should the IALS Estimates be Revised?
    Research Note #1 March 5, 1999 Applied Behavioral & Cognitive Sciences, Inc.
    by Thomas G. Sticht
    Full text available at http://www.nald.ca/fulltext/sticht/resnote.htm
  8. from From International Competitiveness to International Inequality: New Perspectives on Social Justice From the International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS)
    Research Note 12/13/00
    by Thomas G. Sticht
    International Consultant in Adult Education
    Full text available at http://www.nald.ca/fulltext/sticht/dec00/cover.htm
  9. from Ending the myth of the 'Literacy Myth'
    by Stan Jones
    Center for the Study of Adult Literacy, Carleton University
    Full text available at http://www.nald.ca/province/que/litcent/Publication_Products/working/page17.htm
  10. from Literacy, Economy and Society: A Review
    by Brian V. Street, King's College, University of London
    http://www.nald.ca/province/que/litcent/Publication_Products/working/page7.htm
  11. from Ending the myth of the 'Literacy Myth'
    by Stan Jones
    Center for the Study of Adult Literacy, Carleton University
    Full text available at http://www.nald.ca/province/que/litcent/Publication_Products/working/page17.htm

Back to top

1) from Research with Words: Qualitative Inquiry

Most generally speaking, the purpose of qualitative research is to understand human experience to reveal both the processes by which people construct meaning about their worlds and to report what those meanings are. But what particular kinds of information can qualitative studies offer literacy specialists and adult educators?

Such research can reveal how people experience educational activities like literacy classes or work related education programs -- what they value, what they reject, what they learn, how they change. Thereby the studies can tell us something about how and why such programs succeed and fail. This kind of research can also document and characterize the diversity and complexity of literacy activities as they occur in school, work, and daily life, as well as the incentives and disincentives that people perceive for developing and exercising literacy abilities. Thereby we can more fully appreciate the nature of the literacy practices we are attempting to teach. And such studies can introduce us to situations from the points of view of varied participants, bringing to the fore individual perspectives, histories, and proclivities, as well as structures of power that influence what people learn and are able to do. Thereby we can place literacy learning properly in broader historical, sociocultural, and political milieus, learning how learning is influenced by forces outside the classroom.

by Glynda Hull
FOCUS ON BASICS, Vol 1, Issue A • Feb 97
Full text available at http://gseweb.harvard.edu/~ncsall/fob/1997/hull.htm

Back to top


2)  from Understanding Quantitative Research about Adult Literacy

Statistical research is not as formidable as it appears, but it requires a special type of reasoning. Statistical reasoning involves a tight, detailed, and codified logic that can be especially difficult for people who would rather deal in broad strokes and big ideas than with the making of fine distinctions about extremely well focused concepts.

Some people view statistics with a sense of moral indignation at the fact that statistics reduces things of human importance to numbers, and they relate statistics to the power that statistics could give to a "big brother" type of government or to a scorn of bean-counting bureaucrats. In reality, of course, statistical research reduces an object of study no more than a camera reduces the object of a photograph. Statistical reasoning simply represents a highly patterned and highly public way of looking at the world, and, because its details can be readily scrutinized and evaluated, it is often preferred by funding agencies and program evaluators over more subjective and less public ways of reasoning. Like all research methods, it can be used for good or bad purposes.

by Thomas Valentine

FOCUS ON BASICS, Vol 1, Issue A • Feb 97
Full text available at http://gseweb.harvard.edu/~ncsall/fob/1997/valen.htm

Back to top


3) from Reading the Future: A Portrait of Literacy In Canada

Backgrounder

The International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS) is part of a continuing tradition of attempts to measure literacy levels in the adult population by means of surveys and to produce international comparisons. Such research is driven by the search for universals in the relationships between literacy, education and prosperity, which can be used to further the goal of global development.

...

Measuring literacy: More than one gauge

Literacy cannot be narrowly defined as a single skill that enables people to deal with all types of text. People in industrialized countries face many different kinds of written material every day, and they require different skills to understand and use the information. To reflect this complexity, IALS developed three categories of literacy:

1 . Prose literacy: the ability to understand and use information from texts such as editorials, news stories, poems and fiction.

2. Document literacy: the ability to locate and use information from documents such as job applications, payroll forms, transportation schedules, maps, tables and graphs.

3. Quantitative literacy: the ability to perform arithmetic functions such as balancing a chequebook, calculating a tip, or completing an order form.

...

A broad perspective

If economies require increasing numbers of highly skilled workers to expand, then growth will be affected by existing practices of employers, 'individuals and governments: IALS has shown that instead of enlarging the pool of highly skilled workers, the tendency is to increase the skills of the already skilled. The reserve employment pool, made up of the unemployed and those working in declining industrial sectors, is low-skilled. Policies directed towards providing more educational opportunities and increasing skills in that pool must he a necessary part of any industrial growth strategy.

The distribution of literacy is also a good predictor of the magnitude of differences between social groups, making literacy an essential element for promoting social cohesion. Therefore, any view of literacy which is focussed on economic objectives alone is untenable.

Full text available at http://www.nald.ca/NLS/ials/ialsreps/ialsbk1.htm

Back to top


4) from Privileged Literacies: Policy, Institutional Process and the Life of the IALS

The IALS draws on a particular discipline – the psychometric measurement tradition. It uses an information processing model of literacy and attempts to identify levels of literacy skill that are independent of the context of use – the literacy counterpart of the generic and transferable labour skills supposedly possessed by the flexible worker (cf also Bernstein’s (1996: 56) new performance pedagogies). It creates three dimensions of literacy: prose, document and quantitative. In generating test items, the starting points were texts taken from real-life contexts in a range of countries especially NorthAmerica. They include bus timetables, advertisements and consumer instructions. These then underwent various transformations to turn them into test items and those showing cultural bias or linguistic translation problems were dropped. The final test rests on 35 texts, each one used as the basis for several question items. The tests are designed to ensure a broad spread of responses across an arbitrarily fixed set of five levels.

...

The test items cover commercial, financial, media, advertising and entertainment related texts; work-place, including job-seeking; consumer manuals and instructions; transportation-related and a recipe. In general, the test items require reading and formulaic writing responses (such as form-filling) rather than compositional writing which is difficult to subject to standardised scoring. Not included in the test are religious texts, letters (personal or official), greetings/condolences; legal documents, political, government and policy documents; literature (novels, drama or poetry); historical writing; autobiography; humour and satire. This is, therefore, a very limited, standardised ‘generic’ view of literacy presented as a universal standard and from which culturally specific material has been partialled out.

Back to top


5) from Becoming expert: using ethnographies of everyday learning to inform the education of adults

In our project we found vernacular literacies involved in a range of everyday activities, which we roughly classified as (1) organising life (2) personal communication (3) private leisure (4) documenting life (5) sense making and (6) social participation.

...

Firstly, vernacular literacy practices are learned informally. They are acquired in homes and neighbourhood groups, through the everyday perplexities and curiosities of our lives. The roles of novice or learner and expert or teacher are not fixed, but shift from context to context and there is an acceptance that people will engage in vernacular literacies in different ways, sometimes supporting, sometimes requiring support from others.

Secondly, the vernacular literacy practices we identified are rooted in action contexts and everyday purposes and networks. They draw upon and contribute to vernacular knowledge, which is often local, procedural and minutely detailed. Literacy learning and use are integrated in everyday activities and the literacy elements are an implicit part of the activity. which may be mastering a martial art, paying the bills, or finding out about local news. Literacy itself is not a focus of attention, but is used to get other things done.

by Mary Hamilton, Literacy Research Group, Department of Educational Research, Lancaster University, UK.

Back to top


6) IALS Methodology and Validity

from The International Adult Literacy Survey: How well does it represent the literacy abilities of adults?

In the IALS, the performance scales and the self-assessments represent two fundamentally different approaches to assessing literacy abilities. In the performance assessments, literacy is construed as a cognitive ability (latent trait) that makes possible the use of printed materials in various contexts. It is considered that some people have more of this capacity than others, although how much people have or lack may not be consciously apparent to them. Nonetheless, it is assumed that these differences in the amount of capacity can be inferred using people’s performance on various real-world tasks that incorporate the latent trait that is theorized to make possible each person’s performance.

In the self-assessment approach to assessing literacy, literacy is considered as an ability or set of abilities (as in reading, writing, and numeracy in the IALS) that adults are consciously aware of and can perceive well enough to estimate how well their literacy skills permit them to negotiate the literacy demands of different sets of activities at work or in their daily life. This requires that adults are aware both of the demands for literacy in the different contexts that they encounter and of how well their literacy abilities permit them to meet these demands on a recurrent basis.

Clearly, these two different approaches to assessing literacy are based on different implicit theories about literacy and different procedures for measuring literacy. It is also evident from the discrepancies in data that these approaches produce different estimates of how many adults are at risk because of literacy in the various nations that participated in the IALS. These findings raise serious questions about the validity of the different assessments. Is each assessment equally valid as a means of representing the literacy abilities of the adult population? If so, then how should the different results of each method be used?

by Thomas G. Sticht, The Canadian Journal for the Study of Adult Education, Vol, 15, No. 2 November, 2001, 15(2): pp19-36

Back to top


7) from How Many Low Literate Adults Are There in Canada, the United States, and United Kingdom? Should the IALS Estimates be Revised?

Research Note #1 March 5, 1999 Applied Behavioral & Cognitive Sciences, Inc.
Issues in Setting Standards For Adult Literacy.

When the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) was released in 1993 I raised the question as to why it was that 66 to 75 percent of adults in Literacy Level 1, the lowest level, thought they could read and write "well" or "very well," figures that were similar to those found in the later IALS for the three nations under discussion. I noted that this might result from the fact that on the Prose scale of the NALS, people with a score of 200 were assigned to Level 1 because they had an 80 percent probability of being able to do the average task at level 1. However, these same people would be expected to be able to respond correctly to over 45 percent of the average tasks at Level 2, 25 percent of the average tasks at Level 3, and even 15 percent of the average tasks at Level 5, the highest level of literacy. Yet, because they were assigned to Level 1, all competence above that level was denied to them. Similar findings held for Document and Quantitative scales.

...

Both the International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS) and the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) set a criterion of having an 80 percent probability of getting the average item at a given Literacy Level correct to be assigned to that level of skill. This resulted in some 20.7 percent of adults in the United States being classified as in the lowest level of literacy.

However, Andrew Kolstad, the leader of the NALS project at the National Center for Education statistics has argued that the 80 percent probability level is arbitrary (Kolstad, 1996). He calculated the percentage of adults who would be in Level 1 if a criterion of 65 percent, which is used by the National Assessment for Educational Progress (NAEP) for the K-12 system, was used. In this case the percentage of adults scoring below Level 3 dropped to 32, a reduction of some 15 percent.

Kolstad also determined the consequences of using the 50 percent probability criterion used by the widely employed Comprehensive Adult Student Assessment System (CASAS) which is included in the federal government's dissemination network. In this case, only 9 percent were in Level 1, the lowest level of literacy.

...

In more recent analyses, Kolstad (personal communication, January 26, 1999) has demonstrated that the use of the 50 percent probability criterion produces the least errors in predicting whether adults can or cannot perform literacy tasks across the full range of tasks included in the NALS. At the present time in the United States, stimulated by Kolstad’s work, there is considerable debate going on at the National Center for Education Statistics about just which standards should be used for all national assessments, those for school children and adults as well (Kolstad, et al., 1998).

by Thomas G. Sticht

Full text available at http://www.nald.ca/fulltext/sticht/resnote.htm

Question: How many functionally illiterate adults are there in the United States?
Wag's Answer: As many as you would like!

from Has the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) Defamed the Competence of America's Labor Force? Research Note 10 July 2001

by Thomas G. Sticht

http://www.nald.ca/fulltext/sticht/july01/cover.htm

Back to top


8) from From International Competitiveness to International Inequality: New Perspectives on Social Justice From the International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS) Research Note 12/13/00

... the influential Jump Start report of 1989 stated, "There is no way in which the United States can remain competitive in a global economy, maintain its standard of living, and shoulder the burden of the retirement of the baby boom generation unless we mount a forceful national effort to help adults upgrade their basic skills in the very near future (p.iii)." Yet now we find the U.S. pre-eminent in the global economy, with very low unemployment rates internally, and on a par with the world's leading economic western nations in terms of average adult literacy skills. Overall then, there is not much of a basis in the report for arguing that the U. S. is not economically competitive internationally because of low adult literacy. Hence such economically-based arguments are likely to be less influential in the foreseeable future for advocating for adult literacy education.

...

The report goes on to note that ".inequality in the range of literacy scores in North America is also among the highest of the nations surveyed. Especially in he United States, inequality in the distribution of literacy scores on the English test [that is, the NALS] used for the survey is strongly related to economic inequality measured by income differentials between households."

It seems to me, then, that the emphasis of this recent report using IALS data is largely on the inequality of literacy among adults within nations, and the economic consequences of these differences in literacy for adults within a given nation. In many respects, this seems to be somewhat of a change in perspective from the concern for adult literacy as a factor in international competitiveness that has in large part driven the Workforce Investment Act of 1998, to a return to the concern for issues of poverty and the need for individuals to be economically competitive within our nation that led to the enactment of the adult basic education program as part of the War on Poverty's Economic Opportunity Act of 1964. In a sense, with this new report using IALS data, we seem to have gone back from the concerns with international competitiveness of A Nation at Risk of the 1980s and 1990s to the concerns for People at Risk of the 1960s. This might be a more fruitful stance for advocating for the full recognition of the Adult Education and Literacy System (AELS) as the third major, mainstream component of our nation's publicly supported educational structure (K-12, AELS, Higher Education,) for promoting the general health, welfare and prosperity of the nation. It might also augur well for placing workforce development in a more appropriate, tertiary position with regard to its importance as an outcome for adult education and for getting the WIA changed to the Adult Education, Literacy and Workforce Investment Act (AELWIA) when it next comes up for reconsideration.

by Thomas G. Sticht

International Consultant in Adult Education

Full text available at http://www.nald.ca/fulltext/sticht/dec00/cover.htm

Back to top


9) from Ending the myth of the 'Literacy Myth'

One of the strongest claims in IALS is that there is an important relationship between literacy skill and individual economic success.

The evidence for this lies in a number of observations in the study:

  • There is a relationship between income and literacy skill.

For example, in Canada IALS found that over 80% of those at the lowest literacy level had incomes below the median quintile, but just 42% of those at the highest level had incomes this low. Over a quarter of those at the highest level had incomes in the top quintile, but only 5% of those at the lowest level had incomes this high (p. 61 of the report).

  • There is a relationship between labour force attachment and literacy.

For example, in Sweden 23% of those at the lowest literacy level were unemployed vs. just 2% among those at the highest level (p.58).

  • There is a relationship between occupational change and literacy.

For example, in Germany the average score of those working in industries showing the greatest growth was significantly higher than the average score of those working in essentially stagnant industries (p. 65).

...

Literacy ability is a factor in work force participation, it is a factor in social participation. IALS shows this to be the case.

In many ways the IALS message is not new to the literacy field. Anyone who has worked with learners understands that this message is not new to them. It does appear to be new to the people who decide whether there should be funding for literacy programs. The message from the literacy research community in Canada that literacy didn't matter, the same message Street and Graff want us to believe, had nearly succeeded in convincing governments that investments in adult literacy were not worthwhile.

by Stan Jones
Center for the Study of Adult Literacy, Carleton University
http://www.nald.ca/province/que/litcent/Publication_Products/working/page17.htm

Back to top


10) from Literacy, Economy and Society: A Review

The people at lower levels, however, report on themselves as having sufficient literacy skills for their purposes - they do not rate themselves as 'poor' and indeed many rate themselves as 'excellent' (p. 109). This suggests that they apprehend Scribner and Cole's insight that it would be inappropriate to apply the literacy standards of one domain or cultural to those required in another: literacy is specific to context.

The researchers themselves certainly pay lip service to this position and have done a service in moving beyond the dominant view in agency and government surveys that there is a single standard to be applied to all, and that there are just two dimensions in measurement - literacy and illiteracy. They rightly reject the latter term as unhelpful in contemporary society, with its unfortunate connotations of ignorance and deficit. In practice, levels and demands on literacy are varied and are changing all the time, so the researchers instead are attempting to design scales that will capture this complexity and variation. Instead of a simple literacy/illiteracy dichotomy, they measure literacy in three 'domains' - prose literacy, document literacy and quantitative literacy - across which individuals will vary according to experience and context.

However, there are indications throughout the report that the authors, and certainly those who make use of the findings, will in practice resist this call to complexity, and continue to privilege certain kinds of literacy and certain types of knowledge as superior to others. Indeed, that in a sense is the motivation for producing the report in the first place: to tell governments and agencies where their populations are failing so that they can put it right. The language of deficit runs through the report of findings and the commentaries on it.

...

But even before we address these issues, it is significant that the report has reduced the discussion of literacy to these work-oriented issues and to a traditional 'functional' approach to literacy.

The authors' definition of literacy itself reproduces the standard functional view of early UNESCO documents: 'Using printed and written information to function in society: to achieve one's goals, and to develop one's knowledge and potential' (p.14).

This view of literacy has been elaborated, refined and frequently rejected by more sophisticated recent research. But none of this is mentioned in the report, which proceeds as though the definitions and the terms are unproblematic and universally agreed. Similarly the language of deficit already hinted at in the opening reference to 'inadequate levels of literacy' recurs throughout as a leitmotif with constant suggestions of a literacy 'problem,' of 'low' levels, of 'success' or 'failure' in achieving on the tests and of 'remediation' in putting it right.

...

If we instead pose the question of whose interpretations are 'appropriate,' applying a key concept from the Ethnography of Communication tradition, the frame shifts from a monolithic quasi-scientific assumption of truth to a more socially relative recognition that particular discourses and utterances are appropriate to particular social conditions: Scribner and Cole's "specific practices promote specific skills."

If the IALS researchers wish to argue that the conditions set by their survey, and in particular by the test items and the questions respondents were asked about them, lead to certain kinds of response as more 'appropriate,' then they need to characterize that domain more carefully. They need to problematize its assumptions rather than to take them for granted and to indicate something of how other responses would be appropriate in other conditions.

There is a power relation, then, between the researchers and their respondents, on the one hand, and between this particular style of research and other research traditions, on the other. The research team indeed have immense power as the very debate now going on about their findings indicates.

That they do not draw attention to this power but instead write as though their findings are the neutral product of objective scientific inquiry is itself a classic procedure of institutional power. If nothing else, the report will provide excellent data for students interested in the workings of discursive power in late twentieth century Europe and N. America.

by Brian V. Street, King's College, University of London
http://www.nald.ca/province/que/litcent/Publication_Products/working/page7.htm

Back to top


11) from Ending the myth of the 'Literacy Myth'

The specific evidence from the statistical analysis of IALS is that there is simply no general evidence of a situation effect. If previous experience were all that counted, as Street seems to say, then we would expect someone who answered one of the questions based on the bus schedule correctly to answer the rest of the questions on the bus schedule correctly. This doesn't happen. Instead. the best predictor of whether someone can answer a particular question on the bus schedule correctly is how well they answer questions with similar cognitive demands on other, situationally unrelated texts.

But let's assume that Street and Graff are right that literacy is narrowly situationally specific. Then it would seem that IALS has managed to locate and test a situationally specific literacy that is highly related to social and economic well-being.

Street and Graff are sure to claim that we have thus privileged this one kind of literacy. But it is not the IALS researchers who have privileged it, it is society. While we might determine test scores, we don't determine employment, income, social participation or any of the other characteristics we found associated with IALS literacy. It is not for the IALS research team to determine whether it is fair that this one kind of literacy is so valued by society. It would have been negligent of us, however, having discovered these connections not to have reported them.

by Stan Jones

Center for the Study of Adult Literacy, Carleton University
http://www.nald.ca/province/que/litcent/Publication_Products/working/page17.htm


 

placeholder placeholder
placeholder placeholder placeholder
placeholder
 
house - click to link to home page
 
Calendar - Click to link to news page
 
Moon - Click to link to blog
 
Mp3 Player - Click to link to podcasts
 
Search icon - Click to link to search page
 
Sun - Click to link to projects page
 
People - Click to link to friends page
 
Letter - Click to link to subscribe page
 
Link - Click to link to links page
 
Quote Bubble - Click to link to contact page
 
User - Click to link to about us page
placeholder  
placeholder placeholder
placeholder placeholder placeholder
placeholder
placeholder
Valid HTML 4.01! Valid CSS! placeholder Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Licence.